Semiotic Combination in Pan: Evolutionary implications of proto-syntax Heidi Lyn1, Patricia Greenfield2, and E. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh3 1Wildlife Conservation Society, 2UCLA, 3Language Research Center hlyn@wcs.org Over twenty years of research at the Language Research Center have demonstrated that bonobos can learn to comprehend spoken English at the level of a two-and-a-half year old human child without explicit training (E. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh, 1986; E. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh, McDonald, Sevcik, Hopkins, & Rupert, 1986; E. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh et al., 1993; E. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh, Shanker, & Taylor, 1998). In addition, these bonobos could utilize visuographic symbols (lexigrams) on a printed keyboard to communicate with researchers. Earlier research with chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in more formalized language training programs had been less successful (Gardner & Gardner, 1969; Hayes, 1951; Kellogg & Kellogg, 1933; Rumbaugh, 1977; E. S. Savage-Rumbaugh, Rumbaugh, Smith, & Lawson, 1980; Terrace, 1979). Because the research at the Language Research Center included a different species - bonobos (Pan paniscus) and a different rearing environment (including extensive early language enrichment, but no formal training) a co-rearing study was initiated. Rearing a bonobo (Panbanisha) and a chimpanzee (Panpanzee) together demonstrated that the primary source of earlier chimpanzee-bonobo communication differences was rearing environment, rather than species-based genetic differences (Brakke & Savage-Rumbaugh, 1995, 1996; Williams, Brakke, & Savage-Rumbaugh, 1997). This paper focuses on the spontaneous lexigram and gestural symbolic productions of these co-reared apes and compares them to another symbol-competent bonobo and productions from human researchers who were the apes' primary caregivers. Specifically, we report on two-element combinations (two lexigrams or one lexigram plus one gesture) produced by the apes throughout five months of the study period. All productions during the study period were instances of spontaneous two-element combinations, which were then coded according to their semiotic functions, following the procedures in Brown (1973) and Greenfield and Savage-Rumbaugh (1990; 1991). The results demonstrate the construction of communicative conventions: shared norms for combining symbols that were shared by either the two co-reared apes, all three language competent apes or one of the apes and the researchers. For example, all three apes and the human researchers were more likely to order an action before an agent, rather than an agent before an action ("open cooler" rather than "cooler open"). These shared norms were statistical, not absolute, which is true of human children as well (e.g., Bowerman, 1973; Goldin-Meadow & Mylander, 1984). Ordering conventions applied to combinations consisting of lexigram plus gesture, as well as combinations consisting of two lexigrams. These findings are in contrast to earlier reports of language-trained apes which did not find ordering conventions in their productions (Terrace, Petitto, Sanders, & Bever, 1979, 1980). Additionally, a bonobo shared more communicative conventions with his co-reared chimpanzee than with another bonobo who had not shared the same rearing environment - emphasizing the importance of social interactions and early environment in forming communicative conventions, over species-specific predispositions. However, this study offers no evidence to support the knowledge of formal syntactic structure in any of the apes. It is therefore possible that the ability to create communicative conventions at the level of proto-syntax was present in the common ancestor of chimpanzees, bonobos, and humans five million years ago (Byrne, 1995) and subserved the subsequent evolution of communication in each species (Gannon, Holloway, Broadfield, & Braun, 1998). Bowerman, M. (1973). Early syntactic development: A cross-linguistic study with special reference to Finnish. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Brakke, K. E., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1995). The development of language skills in bonobo and chimpanzee - 1. Comprehension. Language and Communication, 15(2), 121-148. Brakke, K. E., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1996). The development of language skills in Pan - II. Production. Language & Communication, 16(4), 361-380. Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Byrne, R. W. (1995). The thinking ape : evolutionary origins of intelligence. Oxford ; New York: Oxford University Press. Gannon, P. J., Holloway, R. L., Broadfield, D. C., & Braun, A. R. (1998). Asymmetry of chimpanzee planum temporale: Humanlike pattern of Wernicke's brain language area homolog. Science, 279, 220-222. Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165, 664-672. Goldin-Meadow, S., & Mylander, C. (1984). Gestural communication in deaf children: The effects and noneffects of parental input on early language development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 49(3-4), 1-121. Greenfield, P. M., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1990). Grammatical combination in Pan paniscus: Processes of learning and invention in the evolution and development of language. In K. R. G. Sue Taylor Parker (Ed.), "Language" and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives. (pp. 540-578): Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, US. Greenfield, P. M., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1991). Imitation, grammatical development, and the invention of protogrammar by an ape. In N. A. Krasnegor, D. M. Rumbaugh, R. L. Schiefelbusch & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Biological and behavioral determinants of language development. (pp. 235-258): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc, Hillsdale, NJ, US. Hayes, C. (1951). The ape in our house ([1st ] ed.). London: Gollancz. Kellogg, W. N., & Kellogg, L. A. (1933). The Ape and the Child. NY., NY.: McGraw-Hill. Rumbaugh, D. M. (1977). Language learning by a chimpanzee: the Lana project. New York: Academic Press. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1986). Ape language: From conditioned response to symbol. New York, NY, US: Columbia University Press. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., McDonald, K., Sevcik, R. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Rupert, E. (1986). Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(3), 211-235. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Murphy, J., Sevcik, R. A., Brakke, K. E., Williams, S. L., & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1993). Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(3-4), v-221. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D. M., Smith, S. T., & Lawson, J. (1980). Reference: The linguistic essential. Science, 210, 922-925. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Shanker, S. G., & Taylor, T. J. (1998). Apes, language, and the human mind. New York: Oxford University Press. Terrace, H. S. (1979). Nim (1st ed.). New York: Knopf : distributed by Random House. Terrace, H. S., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J., & Bever, T. G. (1979). Can an ape create a sentence? Science, 206(4421), 891-902. Terrace, H. S., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J., & Bever, T. G. (1980). On the grammatical capacities of apes. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Children's Language (Vol. 2, pp. 371-496). New York: Gardner Press. Williams, S. L., Brakke, K. E., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1997). Comprehension skills of language-competent and non-language-competent apes. Language and Communication, 17(4), 301-317.