Language emerges only in kin-related groups or if it used to talk to oneself Marco Mirolli*° (mirolli2@unisi.it), Domenico Parisi° *University of Siena °ISTC, National Research Council Language requires the co-evolution of both speakers and understanders. The ability to speak, i.e. to produce useful signals, is useless unless there are individuals that respond to the signals appropriately and, vice-versa, the ability to understand signals is useless unless that are individuals that produce useful signals that systematically co-vary with specific aspects of experience. This implies that if speaking is advantageous for the receivers of signals but not for the individuals that produce them, it is not clear how language may have evolved. Human language is quite unique among animal communicative systems in that it is often used to inform conspecifics about aspects of the environment. It is an open problem whether this use of communication was the principal function for which human language evolved [Bickerton, 2002] or language evolution started for more social reasons (such as facilitating social interaction and social coordination; cf. [Knight et al., 2000], [Dunbar, 1996]). In any case, the evolution of language for informing conspecifics is something that must be explained in that when an individual uses language to inform a conspecific about some aspect of the environment, the advantages for the hearer are clear but those for the speaker aren't. We describe a set of simulations in which a population of individuals lives in a world containing both edible and poisonous mushrooms, with each individual mushroom being different from all others [Cangelosi & Parisi, 1998]. To survive and reproduce an individual must categorize encountered mushrooms as edible or poisonous and respond by approaching and eating the edible mushrooms and avoiding the poisonous ones. The behavior of the individuals is controlled by a neural network. Input units encode the perceptual properties of encountered mushrooms and heard signals; output units encode movements for displacing oneself in the environment and for producing sounds. The networks' connection weights are found using a genetic algorithm. If the sensory capacities of the individuals are spatially restricted such that distant mushrooms can be localized but not recognized, an individual is forced to approach all mushrooms until a mushroom is sufficiently near to be recognized as edible or poisonous. This of course is not very efficient. If a conspecific which is nearer to a distant mushroom produces a signal which co-varies with the mushroom's category, hearing the signal would allow the first individual to approach only edible mushrooms - with a clear increase in efficiency. However, this is a situation in which the production of useful signals benefits the receivers of signals but not their producers. Hence, language should not emerge in these circumstances. Our simulations show in fact that in our simulation scenario language does not emerge if speakers and hearers in linguistic exchanges are randomly selected from the population but it does emerge if the speaker and the hearer are (at least some of the times) genetically similar individuals, i.e., if they are kin. This result is consistent with previous work ([Ackley & Littman, 1994] [Oliphant, 1996]) and might suggest that language (or, at least, language for informing other individuals about the environment) has emerged within small groups of kin-related individuals rather than in larger communities of unrelated individuals. However, even in its early evolutionary stages language may have had not only a social function but also an individual one: to talk to oneself, for example as an aid to memory. In talking to oneself the same individual is both speaker and hearer. Hence, speaking and understanding must necessarily co-evolve. In another simulation we show that language evolves even if speakers and hearers in linguistic exchanges are genetically unrelated individuals provided that the hearer repeats to itself heard signals in order to keep information about mushrooms in its memory. References [Ackley & Littman, 1994] Ackley, D. H. and Littman, M. L. (1994) "Altruism in the evolution of communication". In Brooks R. A. and Maes Pattie (ed.) Artificial Life IV: Proceedings of the International Workshop on the Synthesis and Simulation of Living Systems. MIT Press. [Bickerton, 2002] Bickerton, D. (2002) "Foraging Versus Social Intelligence in the Evolution of Protolanguage". In Alison Wray, (ed.) The Transition to Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Cangelosi & Parisi, 1998] Cangelosi, A. and Parisi, D. (1998). "The emergence of a language in an evolving population of neural networks". Connection Science, 10(2):83--97. [Dunbar,1996] Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996) Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language, London: Faber and Faber. [Knight et al., 2000] Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy M. and Hurford, J. (ed.) (2000) The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: Social Function and the Origins of Linguistic Form. Cambridge University Press. [Oliphant, 1996] Oliphant, M. (1996) "The dilemma of Saussurean communication". Biosystems, 37(1-2):31--38.